In this Hidden Brain episode, psychologist Saul Kassin explores the troubling phenomenon of false confessions. He explains how intense interrogation tactics can manipulate a suspect into doubting their own memory and falsely confessing to crimes they did not commit. For instance, the episode revisits the infamous Central Park Jogger case, where coerced juvenile suspects falsely confessed despite contradictory evidence.
Kassin delves into the interrogation methods taught in police manuals that create high-risk situations for false confessions, such as direct accusations, sleep deprivation, and fake evidence ploys. He also shares findings from his experiments revealing how even mild pressure can induce people to falsely admit to acts they did not commit.
Sign up for Shortform to access the whole episode summary along with additional materials like counterarguments and context.
Saul Kassin highlights how intense police interrogations can lead individuals to falsely confess to crimes, even providing inconsistent details. The high-pressure environment causes some, especially the suggestible, to confess simply to escape the situation.
Kassin discusses tactics like isolation, sleep deprivation, and presenting fake evidence that can manipulate suspects into doubting their own memories and falsely confessing. He cites the Central Park Jogger case, where the confessions of coerced juveniles led to wrongful convictions.
Despite contradictions in confessions, judges and juries often heavily weigh them as proof of guilt, as seen in the Central Park Jogger case. Kassin's research shows suggestible individuals are prone to interrogation manipulation.
According to Kassin's research, police manuals advise accusing suspects directly, isolating them, and using tactics like sleep deprivation and lying about evidence to heighten the risk of false confessions.
Kassin explains how even mild pressure from an authority figure can significantly influence people's actions, citing experiments like Milgram's that demonstrated obedience. His own study found false evidence increased rates of people falsely confessing.
In this notorious case, five black and Latino juveniles were coerced into falsely confessing after intense interrogations, leading to their wrongful conviction despite a lack of DNA evidence. Their convictions were later vacated.
In Kassin's experiments, innocent participants were led to doubt their memories and falsely confess after being accused and presented with false witnesses. His studies also showed people struggled to distinguish between true and false confessions.
The case of Joaquin Robles, who falsely confessed to stealing from AutoZone under interrogation pressure, reveals how such tactics extend beyond criminal justice into corporate investigations.
1-Page Summary
The psychological phenomenon of false confessions is an alarming and complex problem within the justice system, compelling individuals to admit to crimes they did not commit.
Saul Kassin's own childhood experience with a false accusation led to his interest in the psychology behind interrogation and accusations. He discusses how intense interrogations can lead to false confessions, as seen in historical cases where suspects, such as in the early 20th-century murder of Bessie Hollister, confessed under hefty interrogations despite providing false and inconsistent details.
Saul Kassin highlights that the high-pressure environment of police interrogations can lead to false confessions, especially among individuals who are suggestible or those who simply wish to escape the present stressful situation. He exemplifies this with the Central Park Jogger Case, where the boys felt threatened and were subjected to interrogation for lengthy periods. They eventually confessed, believing minimization would help them.
Saul Kassin also describes compliant false confessions – when individuals confess to relieve themselves of the immediate interrogation stress, despite their knowledge of innocence. This stress can force people to confess thinking it would be in their short-term interest to do so, such as ending the interrogation.
In the case of Peter Riley, Kassin reflects on how intense interrogation techniques, including lying about polygraph results, can lead suspects like Riley to doubt their own me ...
The Psychology of False Confessions
Psychologist Saul Kassin delves into the interrogation techniques that can lead to false confessions, often as a result of intense pressure and isolation.
Kassin describes initial interrogation tactics which include directly accusing suspects and insisting they are not lying. He points out that police manuals suggest detectives come into interrogations brandishing a folder, purportedly filled with evidence against the suspect—even if it is untrue—to add pressure.
Based on his research into a designated interrogation manual, Kassin illustrates that police are advised to begin with a direct accusation of guilt. He details how the interrogation process starts with isolating the suspect; bringing them into an interrogation room alone, without friends, family, or lawyers present, creating a situation of isolation. He explains that detectives use this isolation to their advantage, employing tactics like sleep deprivation and interrogations lasting 16 hours or more to heighten the risk of false confessions.
In one instance cited by Kassin, detectives offered a suspect a polygraph test and then lied about the results to convince him he had failed, suggesting he might have repressed his memory of committing the crime. This isolation ensures that suspects have no external checks on reality, allowing officers to lie about evidence without contradiction.
Kassin's discussion extends to how mild pressure from an authority figure can significantly influence an individual's actions, drawing a parallel with Stanley Milgram's obedience experiments, which demonstrated that people would administer shock ...
Techniques Used by Police To Elicit False Confessions
False confessions, a perplexing aspect of criminal justice, occur under various circumstances, and research by psychologists like Saul Kassin sheds light on the misinformation and coercion often involved.
In April 1989, after a woman was brutally attacked in Central Park, the NYPD 'solved' the case within 72 hours by rounding up and interrogating several black and Latino juveniles. Five boys, aged 14 to 16, were coerced into confessing, with four of these confessions videotaped. Despite the lack of matching DNA evidence—several semen samples didn’t match any of the suspects—and the contradictions in their stories, the teenagers were convicted, largely due to the confessions. In 2002, the convictions were vacated following the confession of the actual perpetrator, whose DNA was a match to the crime scene. Saul Kassin, after reviewing the case, was horrified to find no evidence of guilt outside the confessions, which he concluded were fabricated.
The confessions, even with visible nervousness and disparities, were deemed convincing, and judges and juries were swayed. One juror later mentioned that having a confession made other evidence seem unnecessary. Corey Wise, one of the suspects, seemed visibly nervous during video confession proceedings, with his confession reflecting remorse and responsibility.
Saul Kassin’s work has extended to conducting experiments that reveal the susceptibility of suspects to admit guilt falsely. He has helped get innocent people out of prison, emphasizing the societal repercussions, as the true perpetrator often continues committing crimes. Kassin's experiments involved mock crimes where subjects were falsely accused and confederates claimed to witness it, resulting in volunteers doubting their innocence.
Kassin also found in one of his studies involving prison inmates confessing to crimes—one they committed and one they did not—that accuracy rates in distinguishing true from false confessions were only about 54%, r ...
False Confessions: Real-World Case Studies and Examples
Download the Shortform Chrome extension for your browser